He theory of planned behaviour mediate the effects of age, gender and multidimensional health locus of control? Brit J Health Psych. 2002;7:299-316. 21. Sarker AR, Mahumud RA, Sultana M, Ahmed S, Ahmed W, Khan JA. The influence of age and sex on healthcare expenditure of households in Bangladesh. Pamapimod site Springerplus. 2014;3(1):435. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=4153877 tool=pmcentrez renderty pe=abstract. Accessed October 21, 2014. 22. Rahman A, Rahman M. Sickness and remedy: a scenario analysis among the garments workers. Anwer Khan Mod Med Coll J. 2013;four(1):10-14. 23. Helman CG. Culture, Well being and Illness: Cultural Factors in Epidemiology (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: ButterworthHeinemann. 1995;101-145. 24. Chrisman N. The well being searching for procedure: an approach to the organic history of illness. Cult Med Psychiatry. 1977;1:351-377. 25. Ahmed SM, Adams AM, Chowdhury M, Bhuiya A. Gender, socioeconomic development and health-seeking behaviour in Bangladesh. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:361-371. 26. Ahmed SM, Tomson G, Petzold M, Kabir ZN. Socioeconomic status overrides age and gender in figuring out health-seeking behaviour in rural Bangladesh. Bull Planet Health Organ. 2005;83:109-117. 27. Larson CP, Saha UR, Islam R, Roy N. Childhood diarrhoea management practices in Bangladesh: private sector dominance and continued inequities in care. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35:1430-1439. 28. Sarker AR, Islam Z, Khan IA, et al. Estimating the price of cholera-vaccine delivery from the societal point of view: a case of introduction of cholera vaccine in Bangladesh. Vaccine. 2015;33:4916-4921. 29. Nasrin D, Wu Y, Blackwelder WC, et al. Overall health care seeking for childhood diarrhea in building countries: evidence from seven sites in Africa and Asia. Am a0023781 J Trop Med Hyg. 2013;89(1, suppl):3-12. 30. Das SK, Nasrin D, Ahmed S, et al. Well being care-seeking behavior for childhood diarrhea in Mirzapur, rural Bangladesh. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2013;89(suppl 1): 62-68.A major a part of daily human behavior consists of producing choices. When generating these decisions, people usually depend on what motivates them most. Accordingly, human behavior commonly originates from an action srep39151 choice process that takes into account no matter whether the effects resulting from actions match with people’s motives (Bindra, 1974; Deci Ryan, 2000; Locke Latham, 2002; McClelland, 1985). While individuals can explicitly report on what motivates them, these explicit reports inform only half the story, as there also exist implicit motives of which people are themselves unaware (McClelland, Koestner, Weinberger, 1989). These implicit motives have already been defined as people’s non-conscious motivational dispositions that orient, pick and energize spontaneous behavior (McClelland, 1987). Typically, 3 diverse motives are distinguished: the need to have for affiliation, achievement or energy. These motives have been found to predict numerous distinctive types of behavior, such as social interaction fre?quency (Wegner, Bohnacker, Mempel, Teubel, Schuler, 2014), job efficiency (Brunstein Maier, 2005), and ?emotion detection (Donhauser, Rosch, Schultheiss, 2015). Regardless of the fact that lots of studies have indicated that implicit motives can direct and control persons in performing many different behaviors, small is identified concerning the mechanisms via which implicit motives come to predict the behaviors persons pick out to perform. The aim on the present article is usually to provide a first try at elucidating this relationship.